What would be the best method for examining the relationship between age and driving behavior?

The Driving Observation Schedule (eDOS) was developed for use in the Candrive/Ozcandrive five-year prospective study of older drivers to observe the driving behavior of older drivers and monitor changes in driving behaviors over time. The aim of this study is to describe participants’ driving performance during the eDOS driving task and investigate the association between driving performance and cognitive measures.

A subset of Ozcandrive participants (n = 144, 104 male [72%], 40 female [28%], Mean age = 81.49 years, SD = 3.58 years, Range: 76 – 96 years) completed the eDOS driving task. Participants drove to their selected destinations (up to 4 locations), with observations of driving behaviors (both inappropriate and appropriate) recorded for specific driving maneuvers: intersection negotiation, lane-changing, merging, low speed maneuvers and maneuver-free driving. Driving behaviors (e.g. signalling, speed regulation, gap acceptance, lane position, ‘critical errors’) were scored by a trained observer and participants received an overall eDOS driving task score (Maximum = 100 points). Participants also completed a series of cognitive assessments as part of the Year 2 Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocols.

The overall eDOS driving task score was very high (M = 95.77; SD = 5.15; Range = 65.63 – 100). Detailed analyses of participants’ driving behavior revealed a high level of appropriate driving behavior (96%, n = 5,935 maneuvers), with few errors (4%, n = 252 maneuvers). While most participants’ performance on the cognitive assessments was high, some scores were below the criteria for cogntive impairment (BIC) according to conventional benchmarks: (MoCA: M = 26.56, SD = 2.12, Range = 19 – 30, % BIC = 28%; MMSE: M = 29.10, SD = 1.01, Range = 26–30, %BIC = 0%; Trails B: M = 111.66, SD = 43.53, Range = 50 – 301, %BIC = 6%). There was no significant relationship observed between participants’ overall eDOS driving task scores and age (r (144) = −0.17, p > 0.05), and performance on various cognitive assessments including: MoCA (r (144) = 0.07, p > 0.1), MMSE (r (144) = 0.03, p > 0.5), Trail Making Test B (r (144) = 0.09, p > 0.1).

Preliminary analyses of the eDOS driving task revealed a high level of appropriate driving behavior among Ozcandrive older drivers. Despite some participants’ cognitive performance suggesting impairment, participants’ overall eDOS driving task scores were not significantly related to cognitive performance. This finding is consistent with previous research suggesting some older drivers are able to compensate well for age-related cognitive impairment.

Over the next five decades, there will be a substantial increase in both the number and proportion of older people in most industrialized countries (OECD, 2001). With the ageing of the population, it is also anticipated that there will be an increase in older drivers’ licensing rates (Sivak & Schoettle, 2011). Further, the private motor vehicle is likely to remain the principal mode of transport for the emerging cohorts of older drivers who, it is predicted, will be more mobile, travel more frequently, travel greater distances, and will have higher expectations with regard to maintaining personal mobility compared with earlier cohorts (OECD, 2001). Demographic growth, increased licensing rates, and increased motor vehicle use will combine to produce a marked increase in the number of older drivers on the road.

While current figures show that older drivers are involved in few crashes in terms of absolute numbers, they represent one of the highest risk categories for crashes involving serious injury and death per number of drivers and per distance travelled (Langford & Koppel, 2006; Koppel, Bohensky, Langford & Taranto, 2011), largely due to their physical fragility and hence increased vulnerability to injury (Augenstein, 2001; Dejeammes & Ramet, 1996; Evans, 1991; Li, Braver & Chen, 2003; Mackay, 1998; Padmanaban, 2001; Viano, Culver, Evans, Frick, & Scott, 1990). In addition, the high prevalence of age-related medical conditions and functional impairments among older drivers also contributes to their heightened crash risk (Marshall, 2008), and thus there is a need for accurate assessments, policies and guidelines regarding fitness to drive in the context of age, as well as specific medical conditions (Vlahodimitrakou, Charlton, Langford, Koppel, Di Stefano, Macdonald, Smith, Porter, Gelinas, Mazer, Vrkljan, & Marshall, In Press).

This is a complex policy topic; not all older drivers are unsafe, and while the crash risk associated with ageing may have implications for older drivers at a population level, this may not be reflected at an individual level (Anstey, Wood, Lord, & Walker, 2004). The current paper describes the baseline driving behavior of a cohort of older drivers, using an on-road driving task, the Driving Observation Schedule (eDOS) and investigates the association between driving performance on the eDOS driving task and cognitive measures from the Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocols.

The Candrive/Ozcandrive study is a five year, multi-centre international research program with the core objective of promoting older drivers’ safe mobility (see Marshall, Man-Son-Hing, Bédard, Charlton, Gagnon, Gélinas, Koppel, Korner-Bitensky, Langford, Naglie, Rapoport, Mazer, Myers, Polgar, Porter, Tuokko, Vrkljan, In Press). The Candrive/Ozcandrive study involves 928 drivers aged 70 years and over in Canada and 302 drivers aged 75 years and older in Australia and New Zealand. Using a longitudinal study design, the project is tracking this cohort of older drivers for up to five years, assessing changes in their functional abilities, driving patterns and driving performance. The primary purpose is to determine and validate a screening test (Decision Rule) to identify potentially at-risk drivers (Marshall et al., In Press). Participants’ driving behavior, including trip duration and distance, is recorded through an in-car recording device (ICRD) installed in the participant’s own vehicle at the beginning of the study. In addition, a range of psychometric measures of functional ability, medical conditions and abilities related to driving are documented annually.

The eDOS driving task will be used to study driving behaviors of the Ozcandrive cohort (n = 250 in Australia) and a sub-sample (up to 150) of Candrive participants.

The Driver Observation Schedule (eDOS) is an on-road driving task, designed initially for use in the Ozcandrive study, to evaluate older drivers’ driving behavior in order to monitor changes in individual driving behaviors over time (Koppel, Charlton, Langford, Di Stefano, Macdonald, Mazer, Gelinas, Vrkljan & Marshall, 2012; Vlahodimitrakou, et al., In Press). Additionally, it was expected that such a tool could supplement the (potentially rare) primary outcome measures of crashes (and police-recorded infringements/violations of traffic safety rules and regulations) for validation of the screening test.

In developing the eDOS driving task, key criteria were that it should reflect drivers’ everyday driving and be feasible (in light of both time and resources) to sustain within the multi-site, longitudinal study. More specifically, eDOS driving task requirements were:

  • observation of ‘natural’ driving with no intervention/instruction by the observer;

  • conducted in driver’s own vehicle since Lundberg & Hakamies-Blomquist (2003) found significant differences in driving test outcomes for drivers doing an on-road test using a test vehicle versus their own car);

  • conducted over routes familiar to and chosen by the older driver;

  • takes approximately 20–25 minutes to complete;

  • rates behaviors specifically associated with older driver safety.

The Person-Environment (P-E) Fit theory of driving competence (Willis, 2000) and Michon’s Model of Driver Behavior (Michon, 1985) were influential in determining the nature of the eDOS driving task, particularly its emphasis on driver-selected, familiar routes to afford the possibility of observing drivers’ competency in environments presenting the types and levels of demand encountered in their everyday driving (see Vlahodimitrakou, et al., In Press). In addition, item selection and operationalization of the eDOS driving task was based on older driver crash epidemiology (e.g., Catchpole, et al., 2005; Fildes, et al., 1994; Korner-Bitensky, et al., 2006; Langford & Koppel, 2006), older driver self-regulatory behavior (e.g., Charlton, et al., 2006; Baldock, et al., 2006), and published driving measures (e.g., Di Stefano & Macdonald, 2003; 2006; Dobbs, et al., 1998; Galski, et al., 1992; Hunt, et al., 1997; Justiss, 2005; Kowalski, et al., 2010; Malloon & Wood, 2004; Odenheimer, et al., 1994; Ott, et al., 2012). Based on these findings, six categories of driving behaviors were identified for inclusion in the final eDOS driving task: a) Observation of Road Environment; b) Signaling; c) Speed Regulation; d) Gap Acceptance; e) Road-Rule Compliance; and f) Vehicle/Lateral Lane Positioning. The behaviors are scored during driving maneuvers, as appropriate or inappropriate: intersection negotiation, lane-changing, merging, low speed maneuvers and maneuver-free driving (i.e., straight travel path).

Vlahodimitrakou and colleagues (In Press) evaluated the inter-rater reliability, feasibility and acceptability of the eDOS driving task on a sub-sample of thirty-three Ozcandrive participants (20 male [61%], 13 female [39%], Mean age = 80.12 years, SD = 3.39, Range: 75–88 years). The authors reported that the eDOS driving task was possible to implement in participants’ own vehicles, could be scored reliably and consistently (ICC = 0.905, CI 95% 0.747–0.965, p < 0.0001; r (18) = .83, p <. 05), was practical in terms of duration, and was acceptable to participants. More recently, Koppel and colleagues revised the eDOS driving task, including: i) an electronic scoresheet to record and score driving behavior, and ii) installation of video recording equipment in participants’ vehicles to capture images of the driver and the forward driving environment throughout the drive. Based on a sub-sample of 96 Ozcandrive participants, they reported that it was possible to observe and score detailed driving behavior during intersection negotiation, lane-changing, merging and maneuver-free driving, and that despite the revisions, the eDOS driving task was feasible to use in participants’ own vehicles, and was acceptable to older participants.

The aim of this study was to describe participants’ driving performance during the eDOS driving task and investigate the association between driving performance on the eDOS driving task and cognitive measures from the Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocols.

One hundred and forty four Ozcandrive participants (104 male [72.22%], 40 female [27.78%], Mean age = 81.49 years, SD = 3.58, Range: 76 years – 96 years) completed the eDOS driving task. All participants were required to meet the following inclusion criteria: a) aged 75 years or older; b) held a valid driver’s licence; c) drove at least four times per week, and d) did not have an absolute contraindication to driving, as defined by the Austroads Fitness to Drive Guidelines (2006).

The eDOS driving task

Participants’ driving behavior was observed by a trained observer who scored the driving behavior using an electronic scoresheet (see Figure 1). Images of the driver and forward driving environment were also recorded throughout the eDOS driving task1.

What would be the best method for examining the relationship between age and driving behavior?

Example of eDOS scoresheet

Whilst the driving route itself was not standardized, driving behaviors were observed and documented using standardized procedures for intersection negotiation, lane-changing, merging, maneuver-free driving and low speed maneuvering. Six categories of driving behaviors (appropriate and inappropriate) were scored and recorded for each intersection negotiation, lane change and merge: a) observation of road environment, b) signaling, c) speed regulation, d) gap acceptance, e) road-rule compliance, and f) vehicle/lateral lane positioning. The definitions for inappropriate driving behaviors are presented in Table 1. Route complexity was recorded in terms of traffic density, speed zone and number of road lanes.

Definitions for inappropriate driving behavior

Driving BehaviorSpecific ErrorExplanation
Observation of Road Environment: Maintaining awareness of surroundings & road environmentNo Mirror UseNon-use of rear-/ side-view mirrors
No lookingFailure to look ahead/left/right before proceeding through intersection
Signaling: Ability to signal intention to negotiate an intersectionInappropriateFailure to use signal/leaving signal on after negotiating intersection/Use of incorrect signal
Speed Regulation: Adhering to posted speed limits, & regulating speed consistent with road/traffic conditionsToo FastDriving over speed limit or at dangerous speed for maneuver
Too SlowDriving too slowly; (consistently; a sign of over cautiousness)
Gap acceptance: Making safe judgments about presence of other vehicles & selecting a suitably risk-free point to pull into line of traffic, or cross one or more lanes of trafficMissed OpportunityBeing overcautious/missing opportunities when selecting gap
Unsafe GapSelecting unsafe gap
Failure to YieldFailing to yield (give right of way)
Hitting CurbHitting side curb
Road-Rules Compliance: Ability to follow & appropriately respond to road signs, & not cross pavement markingsNon Compliance Light/SignFailing to comply with road sign/traffic light
Crossing PavementCrossing a pavement marking to the extent of disturbing other road users
Vehicle/Lane Positioning: Position of vehicle whilst moving or stopped, in accordance with side lane markings on a motorwayOut of LaneDrifting out of lane (with or without marked lanes)
Hitting CurbHitting side curb
Inappropriate Following DistanceDriving too close to vehicle in-front

The observer was also required to document the occurrence of ‘critical errors’, defined as errors which result in: 1) the observer terminating the eDOS driving task; 2) the vehicle being involved in a crash or near-crash; and/or 3) the observer using verbal prompts either to prevent an error escalating in severity or to correct the error.

The total eDOS driving task score (maximum 100 points) was derived as: the total number of driving maneuvers completed appropriately, less one point for each error performed during maneuver-free driving, less two points for each critical error, divided by the total number of maneuvers observed, multiplied by 100. The computation of the total eDOS driving task score was adapted from an approach commonly employed in driving assessment research (see Di Stefano & Macdonald, 2003; Odenheimer et al., 1994).

Post-drive Survey

A post-drive survey comprising four items was developed to assess drivers’ perceptions of the eDOS driving task experience: 1) Participants were asked to rate the quality of their overall driving during the eDOS driving task, 2) the difficulty of the eDOS driving task compared with their everyday driving, 3) their familiarity with the selected route during the eDOS driving task, and 4) their level of comfort with being observed while undertaking the eDOS driving task.

Cognitive Performance Measures

Sample characteristics (gender and age), as well as scores on selected cognitive measures (Montreal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA], a brief cognitive assessment where scores range from 0 to 30, with scores < 26 suggestive of mild cognitive impairment [Nasreddine et al., 2005]; Mini-Mental Status Examination [MMSE], a brief cognitive assessment, where scores range from 0 to 30, with scores < 24 suggestive of cognitive impairment [Folstein, Folstein & McHugh, 1975], and Trail Making Test B [Moses, 2004], a timed executive functioning task, where scores > 180 secs have been associated with increased crash risk [see Staplin, Gish, & Wagner, 2003]) from the Candrive/Ozcandrive protocol were used to investigate the relationship between cognitive performance and driving performance on the eDOS driving task.

Ethics Approval was obtained from the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (MUHREC), and all participants provided written informed consent.

All participants underwent an annual assessment that incorporated a range of psychometric measures of functional ability, medical conditions and abilities related to driving (see Marshall et al., In Press). The annual assessment was conducted up to eight months before participants completed the eDOS driving task.

The eDOS driving task was implemented by a single trained observer who underwent six hours of training with an Occupational Therapist Driving Expert in both classroom and on-road environments. Training included familiarization with the eDOS observation and recording procedures, general principles of driving assessment and the video recording equipment setup and installation.

The eDOS driving task commenced from each participant’s home and was conducted on routes familiar to and chosen by the participants. Prior to commencement of the trip, participants were asked to nominate up to four nearby locations to which they regularly drive, and to devise a driving route commencing and ending at home and linking the nominated destinations within a 20–25 minute round trip.

Throughout the eDOS driving task, the observer documented driving behavior for maneuvers specified in the eDOS driving task as they occurred during the agreed route, scoring these against the eDOS driving task criteria. The observations were conducted in the participants’ vehicle with the observer seated in the rear left seat (i.e., behind the front passenger seat) to ensure that critical aspects of driving behavior were observable (see Fox, Bowden, & Smith, 1998).

The eDOS driving task was designed to study driving behaviors that reflect everyday driving, so drivers were encouraged to behave as they normally would, including listening to the radio or music etc. They were also permitted to have their regular passenger travel with them, if they so chose, and such passengers were allowed to assist with navigation. In addition, several eDOS driving task appointments were rescheduled due to bad weather when the participant(s) reported that they never drove in bad weather (e.g., rain, hail, etc.).

eDOS driving task descriptives

The average duration for the eDOS driving task for all participants was 24 min, 54 s (SD = 7 min 59 s, range 12 min, 39 sec – 1 hour, 5 min, 57 sec), with the majority of participants completing the eDOS driving task within 5 minutes of the target range of 20–25 minutes (90%), and the average distance driven during the eDOS driving task was 13.32 km (SD = 5.25, Range = 5 km – 29 km) (Note: Due to technical difficulties with tablet, driving distance was only available for n = 92 participants). Most participants drove to one or two nominated nearby destinations (1 destination = 47%, 2 destinations = 36%, 3 destinations = 15%, 4 destinations = 2%), with many participants nominating a shopping centre or local shopping strip as one of their destinations (46%). Participants also nominated sports/hobbies (25%), family or friends’ houses (14%), their medical centers or practices (5%), and their Church (3%).

eDOS driving task performance

The average eDOS driving task score (Maximum = 100) was high (M = 95.77; SD = 5.15; Range = 65.63 – 100). A summary of the frequency of driving maneuvers observed during the eDOS driving task, including intersection negotiation, lane changing and merging, is presented in Table 2.

Summary of observed driving maneuvers during the eDOS driving task

Mean (SD) ObservedRange Observed
Intersections (all)
Traffic lights (with arrow)
  Turning left0.19 (0.43)0–2
  Turning right1.24 (1.12)0–5
Traffic lights (no arrow)
  Turning left1.03 (1.08)0–4
  Turning right0.99 (0.99)0–4
  Straight through12.41 (8.74)0–47
Controlled Intersection (No traffic light)
  Turning left2.19 (1.43)0–7
  Turning right0.98 (0.96)0–5
  Straight through0.22 (0.53)0–2
Uncontrolled Intersection
  Turning left3.68 (2.31)0–13
  Turning right3.24 (1.88)0–8
  Straight through0.03 (0.16)0–1
Roundabout (any maneuver)3.34 (3.31)0–18
U-turn, any intersection0.21 (0.44)0–2
Lane Changes7.98 (4.95)1–31
Merges1.44 (1.13)0–4

Preliminary analyses of participants’ driving behavior during intersection negotiation, lane changing and merging is presented below.

Intersection negotiation

The driving behavior, including both appropriate and inappropriate behavior, observed during intersection negotiation is shown in Table 3.

Instances of appropriate and inappropriate driving behavior observed during intersection negotiation

Intersection Negotiation BehaviorPercentage (Number)
Total Intersectionsn = 4, 312
  Appropriate Behavior97 (4,181)
  Inappropriate Behavior3 (131)
Frequency of Inappropriate Behavior observed:
Inappropriate Signaling64
Speed regulationToo fast34
Too slow0
Lateral lane positionOut of lane7
Hitting curb19
Observation of road environmentNo mirror use1
No looking2
Gap acceptanceMissed opportunity3
Unsafe gap6
Failure yielding0
Road rule complianceNon compliance lights/signs11
Crossing pavement0

As shown in Table 3, the most common errors observed during intersection negotiation included: inappropriate signaling (n = 64), driving too fast (n = 34), hitting the curb (n = 19) or non compliance with road rules (n = 11).

The driving behavior, including both appropriate and inappropriate behavior, observed during different intersection types is shown in Table 4. Due to technological difficulties, intersection type was only available for 98 percent of the intersections observed (n = 4,312).

Instances of appropriate and inappropriate driving behavior observed during different intersection types

Intersection TypePercentage Appropriate (Number)Percentage Inappropriate (Number)
Traffic lights (with arrow)
  Turning left89 (25)11 (3)
  Turning right98 (175)2 (4)
Traffic lights (no arrow)
  Turning left100 (150)0 (0)
  Turning right97 (140)3 (5)
  Straight through100 (1794)0 (8)
Controlled Intersection (No traffic light)
  Turning left93 (294)7 (21)
  Turning right96 (135)4 (6)
  Straight through97 (31)3 (1)
Uncontrolled Intersection
  Turning left95 (508)5 (24)
  Turning right98 (461)2 (11)
  Straight through100 (4)0 (0)
Roundabout (any maneuver)91 (437)9 (45)
U-turn, any intersection90 (27)10 (3)
Total97 (4,181)3 (131)

As shown in Table 4, participants were most likely to make errors while turning left at a traffic light with an arrow (11%), making a U-turn (10%), negotiating a roundabout (9%) and turning left at a controlled intersection (e.g., stop sign or giveway/yield sign, 7%). Note, in Australia, right turns are made across traffic.

Lane changing

The driving behavior, including both appropriate and inappropriate behavior, observed during lane changes is shown in Table 5.

Instances of appropriate and inappropriate driving behavior observed during lane changes

Lane Changing BehaviorPercentage (Number)
Total Lane changes codedn = 1,149
  Appropriate Behavior93 (1,074)
  Inappropriate Behavior7 (77)
Frequency of Inappropriate Behavior observed:
Inappropriate Signaling69
Speed regulationToo fast2
Too slow0
Vehicle positioningOut of lane1
Hitting curb0
Inappropriate following distance0
Observation of road environmentNo mirror use1
No looking3
Gap acceptanceMissed opportunity0
Unsafe gap1
Failure yielding0

As shown in Table 5, the most common errors observed during lane changes included: signaling (n = 69), not looking (n = 3) or driving too fast (n = 2).

Merging

The driving behavior, including both appropriate and inappropriate behavior, observed during merging is shown in Table 6.

Instances of appropriate and inappropriate driving behavior observed during merges

Merging BehaviorPercentage (Number)
Total Merges codedn = 207
  Appropriate Behavior90 (187)
  Inappropriate Behavior10 (20)
Frequency of Inappropriate Behavior observed:
Inappropriate Signaling16
  Speed regulationToo fast0
Too slow0
Vehicle positioningOut of lane0
Hitting curb0
Inappropriate following distance0
Observation of road environmentNo mirror use0
No looking2
Gap acceptanceMissed opportunity0
Unsafe gap2
Failure yielding0

As shown in Table 6, errors observed during merging included: inappropriate signaling (n = 16), not looking (n = 2) or choosing an unsafe gap (n = 2).

Critical Errors

Seven critical errors were observed across the 144 eDOS drives:

  • - One participant backed into the mailbox while reversing out of his driveway; this participant also drifted out of his lane, almost colliding with another car while trying to shut his door on the freeway and performed an illegal U-turn within the central business district (CBD) while waiting on tram lines;

  • - Two participants performed unsafe merges by speeding up and forcing other cars to slow down and give them right of way;

  • - One participant performed a lane change in an unsafe gap, and

  • - One participant failed to notice a pedestrian behind his car while reversing out of a car park at a shopping centre. The pedestrian took evasive action to avoid a collision.

Relationship between driving performance on eDOS driving task and cognitive measures

Participants’ performance on cognitive measures from the Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocol was quite high according to conventional benchmarks for cognitive impairment (see Table 7).

Participants’ performance on selected cognitive measures

Cognitive measureMean (SD)RangeBenchmark for cognitive impairment% (N) below impariment criteria
MOCA26.56 (2.22)19 – 30< 2628% (41)
MMSE29.10 (1.01)26 – 30< 240% (0)
Trails B111.66 sec (43.53)50 – 301 sec> 180 sec6% (9)

A series of bivariate correlations were undertaken to investigate the relationship between driving performance on eDOS driving task and age and cognitive measures. There was no significant relationship observed between participants’ overall eDOS driving task scores and age (r (144) = −0.17, p > 0.05), and their performance on various cognitive measures including: MoCA (r (144) = 0.07, p > 0.1), MMSE (r (144) = 0.03, p > 0.5), and Trail Making Test B (r (144) = 0.09, p > 0.1). In addition, there was no significant difference in overall eDOS driving task scores for participants who scored above and below the cognitive impairment criteria for the MoCA (Above: M = 96.06, SD = 5.34; Below: M = 95.03, SD = 4.63, t(142) = −1.084, p > 0.1).

Post-drive Survey

When asked about their driving performance during the eDOS driving task compared with their everyday driving (where 1 = much better, 3 = about the same, 5 = much worse), most participants rated their eDOS driving task performance as ‘about the same’ as their everyday driving (97%). Several participants (3%, n = 3) indicated that their driving performance was ‘better’ during the eDOS driving task.

Participants were then asked to rate the difficulty of the eDOS driving task compared with the difficulty of their everyday driving (where 1 = much less difficult, 3 = about the same, 5 = much more difficult). Most participants rated the difficulty of the eDOS driving task as ‘about the same’ as their everyday driving (74%). Interestingly, 21 percent of participants rated the eDOS driving task as ‘a little less difficult’ and five percent rated the eDOS driving task as ‘a little more difficult’ compared with their everyday driving.

Participants were asked to rate their level of familiarity with the route undertaken during the eDOS driving task (where 5 = completely familiar, 3 = neither familiar or unfamiliar, 1 = completely unfamiliar). Most participants reported that they were ‘highly familiar’ (94%) or ‘familiar’ (6%) with the eDOS route.

Finally, participants were asked to rate their level of comfort with being observed during the eDOS driving task (where 5 = completely at ease, 1 = completely uneasy). Most participants reported that they were ‘completely at ease’ (73%) being observed during the eDOS driving task and the remaining participants reported that they were ‘at ease’ (27%).

The aim of this study was to describe participants’ driving performance during the eDOS driving task and investigate the association between driving performance on the eDOS driving task and cognitive measures from the Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocol.

The findings of the current study demonstrate that a suitable eDOS driving task could be formulated from drivers’ self-nominated destinations (Mean duration of eDOS driving task: 24 min, 54 s, SD = 7 min 59 s), with the majority of drivers completing the eDOS driving task within five minutes of the targeted range of 20–25 minutes (90%) and almost half of the destinations nominated by participants included shopping centers (46%).

Consistent with the findings of the pilot study described by Vlahodimitrakou and colleagues (In Press), overall eDOS driving task scores (Maximum = 100) for a sample of Ozcandrive participants (n = 144) was very high (M = 95.77; SD = 5.15). Detailed analyses of participants’ driving behaviour revealed high levels of appropriate driving behaviour during intersection negotiation (97%), lane changing (93%) and merging (90%). Compared with other studies (e.g. Di Stefano & Macdonald, 2003) drivers’ error rates were very low. This difference may reflect the relatively better health of the present sample compared with that of Di Stefano and Macdonald (2003), in which drivers had been referred to the licensing authority for review because their driving competence was in question. Interestingly, the majority of inappropriate behaviours observed in the current study across all driving manoeuvres were signalling errors. A proportion of behaviours could potentially impact more on safety, including poor gap choices at intersections and inappropriate speed choices (too fast) for all three manoeuvre categories.

In terms of intersection negotiation, the most common errors observed included: inappropriate signalling, driving too fast or hitting the curb. The types of errors observed at intersections in the current study are consistent with those observed in other recent intersection error studies with younger, middle-aged and older drivers (Gstalter & Fastenmeier, 2010; Young, Salmon & Lenne, 2012). When driving errors were compared across intersection types, participants in the current study were most likely to make errors while turning left at a traffic light with an arrow (11%), making a U-turn (10%), negotiating a roundabout (9%) and turning left at a controlled intersection (e.g., stop sign or giveway/yield sign, 7%). This finding is somewhat consistent with the findings recently reported by Gstalter and Fastenmeier (2010) who reported that the highest errors occurred for non-signalised intersections and roundabouts for all drivers. In particular, the authors noted that older drivers were most likely to commit inappropriate signalling errors (false or missing) at roundabouts, especially at the exit.

Undergoing a driving ‘evaluation’ in which driving performance is scrutinized can often be very stressful for older individuals. Therefore, ensuring that drivers are at ease with being observed and comfortable with demands of the route is a key requirement for eDOS driving task acceptability (Vlahodimitrakou et al., In Press). From a road safety viewpoint, it is also desirable that the eDOS driving task reflect everyday driving. While the researchers were careful to emphasize to participants that the eDOS driving task was not a test, we were conscious that the task was somewhat contrived in its destination-chaining requirements (primarily to ensure that trip duration was not excessive), and that presence of an observer may cause discomfort and/or alter performance. Results of the post-drive survey showed that most participants rated their overall driving during the eDOS driving task as ‘about the same when compared with their normal driving’ (97%) and that, even though they knew they were being observed, they were ‘completely at ease’ (73%). It seems likely that the use of drivers’ own vehicles contributed to their feelings of ‘ease’ with the eDOS driving task procedure, although there is no direct evidence of this. Research by Lundberg and Hakamies-Blomqvist (2003) reported higher fail rates for medically-referred drivers using a test vehicle compared with drivers using their own vehicles. The authors attributed the result to drivers’ need to adapt to an unfamiliar vehicle, which imposed an additional cognitive load that compromised their driving ability. Overall, present findings suggest that the participants believed that their performance on the eDOS driving task was representative of their everyday driving. This suggests that eDOS driving task has a high level of face validity in reflecting drivers’ everyday driving. Given the increasing international interest and use of modified (local area) licenses (Langford & Koppel, 2011), the eDOS driving task also offers a promising approach for test construction.

Participants’ overall eDOS driving task scores were not significantly related to their age or their performance on selected cognitive measures from the Candrive/Ozcandrive assessment protocol. This is not surprising, given that only a small proportion of the sample’s performance may be suggestive of cognitive impairment according to conventional benchmarks (e.g., 28% scored < 26 on the MoCA [Nasreddine et al., 2005]; 0% scores < 24 on the MMSE [Folstein et al., 1995]; 6% scored > 180 sec on Trails Making Part B [Staplin et al., 2003]). It will be important to explore the potential relationship between performance on the eDOS driving task and cognitive performance with the full sample (n = 250), as well as to explore potential age-related cognitive changes over the 5 year period of the cohort study.

In addition, as noted by Vlahodimitrakou and colleagues (In Press), several issues relating to the coding and computation of eDOS driving task scores need further investigation. First, coding of appropriate/inappropriate performance for the driving maneuvers within the eDOS driving task relied on subjective judgments of the trained observer. However, we provided a detailed data dictionary and instruction manual to guide the coding of observations and to improve objectivity of judgments. Inter-rater reliability of the eDOS driving task is currently being explored.

Second, the computation of the total eDOS driving task score was adapted from an approach commonly employed in driving assessment research (see Di Stefano & Macdonald, 2003; Odenheimer et al., 1994). Weighting of errors is controversial due to the multitude of factors that contribute to the level of “severity” assigned to a given error and the possible range of safety implications resulting from the error (Dobbs et al, 1998, Di Stefano & Macdonald, 2006). For example, Justiss and Stav (2006) and others (e.g., Dobbs et al., 1998) used a more complex rating of errors and applied heavier penalty to critical errors. It should be noted that only seven critical errors were observed and therefore, regardless of the weighting assigned here, its contribution to overall eDOS driving task scores in the current study was minimal. Future research is proposed to explore weighting of errors and implications for safety, as discussed below.

Third, a limitation of the total eDOS driving task score is that it can be interpreted only in relative terms, meaning that any given driver has demonstrated more or fewer errors over time (e.g., over the duration of the Ozcandrive/Candrive study), or more/fewer errors than another driver. It is also possible to use eDOS driving task observations to describe driver performance in terms of patterns of maneuvers made and types of errors (e.g., gap acceptance, signaling). In addition, there is an opportunity for refinement of the current scoring approach to enable determination of how much a given difference in scores matters from a safety viewpoint. Recent work by Classen and colleagues (Classen, Shechtman, Awadzi, Joo, & Lanford, 2010) demonstrated a hierarchy of importance of errors in predicting crash-related injury with highest probability for injury associated with lane maintenance, yielding, and gap acceptance errors; moderate associations with speed regulation; and vehicle positioning and adjustment to stimuli conferring a low probability. Their findings will be useful for advancing the measurement of driving errors and associated safety effects. It will be important that further analyses are conducted to explore the potential refinement of the present scoring system before application to the rest of the sample.

Preliminary analyses of the eDOS driving task revealed a high level of appropriate driving behavior among Ozcandrive older drivers (96%). Despite some participants’ cognitive performance suggesting cognitive impairment, participants’ overall eDOS driving task scores were not significantly related to cognitive performance. This finding is consistent with previous research which suggests that at least some older drivers are able to compensate well for age-related cognitive impairment. It should be noted that these findings are based on a subsample of Ozcandrive participants. Future analyses are planned, including: 1) using a range of measures collected through the Candrive/Ozcandrive annual assessments to investigate whether changes in functional performance or health might predict changes in individuals’ driving performance on the eDOS driving task over time, and 2) using participants’ real-world driving data to investigate whether their driving performance during the eDOS driving task is representative of their everyday driving performance.

The Candrive II study was funded by a Team Grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) entitled “The CIHR Team in Driving in Older Persons (Candrive II) Research Program” (grant 90429). The Candrive eDOS subproject is funded by AUTO21 – A Network of Centres of Excellence (NCE). Ozcandrive (including the Ozcandrive eDOS subproject) is funded by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant (LP 100100078) to the Monash University in partnership with La Trobe University, VicRoads, Victorian Government Department of Justice and Victoria Police, the Transport Accident Commission, Road Safety Trust New Zealand and Eastern Health. The authors gratefully acknowledge the invaluable contribution of Amy Allen, Lorraine Atkinson, Louise Beasley, Russ Boag, Matthew Catchlove, Cara Dawson, Johan Davydov, Yik-Xiang Hue, Elizabeth Jacobs, Duncon Joiner, Jason Manakis, Kevin Mascarenhas and Jarrod Verity, who assisted with the Ozcandrive data collection, as well as the eDOS and camera implementation. We thank the Candrive/Ozcandrive teams and the older driver participants, without whose valuable input and commitment, this research would not be possible.

1Please note that the video images were not used for scoring participants’ driving behaviour in this manuscript.

  • Anstey KJ, Wood J, Lord S, Walker JG. Cognitive, sensory and physical factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;25:45–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Augenstein J. Differences in clinical response between the young and the elderly. Paper presented at the Aging and Driving Symposium, Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine; Des Plaines, IL. 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • Austroads . Medical standards for licensing and clinical management guidelines. A resource for health professionals in Australia. Sydney, Australia: 2006. Assessing fitness to drive for commercial and private vehicle drivers. [Google Scholar]
  • Baldock MRJ, Mathias J, McLean J, Berndt A. Self-Regulation of Driving and Older Drivers’ Functional Abilities. Clinical Gerontologist. 2006;30:53–70. [Google Scholar]
  • Ball KK, Roenker DL, Wadley VG, Edwards JD, Roth DL, McGwin G, Jr, Raleigh R, Joyce JJ, Cissell GM, Dube T. Can high-risk older drivers be identified through performance-based measures in a Department of Motor Vehicles setting? Journal of American Geriatric Society. 2006;54:77–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Catchpole J, Styles T, Pyta V, Imberger K. Exposure and accident risk among older drivers (Report No. 366) Australia: ARRB Group Ltd; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • Charlton JL, Oxley J, Fildes B, Oxley P, Newstead S, Koppel S, O’Hare M. Characteristics of older drivers who adopt self-regulatory driving behaviours. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2006;9(5):363–373. [Google Scholar]
  • Classen S, Shechtman O, Awadzi KD, Joo Y, Lanford DN. Traffic violations versus driving errors of older adults: Informing clinical practice. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2010;64:233–241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Davey RJ, Jamieson S. The validity of using the mini mental state examination in NICE dementia guidelines. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2004;75(2):343–344. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dejeammes M, Ramet M. Aging process and safety enhancement of car occupants. Paper presented at the 15th International Technical Conference on the Enhanced Safety of Vehicles; Melbourne, Australia. 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Di Stefano M, Macdonald W. Assessment of older drivers: Relationships among on-road errors, medical conditions and test outcome. Journal of Safety Research. 2003;34:415–429. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Di Stefano M, Macdonald W. On-the-road evaluation of driving performance. In: Pellerito J, editor. Driver rehabilitation and community mobility: principles and practice. St. Louis, Missouri: Elsevier Mosby; 2006. pp. 255–274. [Google Scholar]
  • Di Stefano M, Macdonald W. Design of on-road test routes: validity issues for assessment of functionally impaired drivers. Proceedings of the Road Safety, Research, Policing and Education Conference November 10–12; Sydney: Australian College of Road Safety; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • Dobbs A, Hellen R, Schopflocker D. A comparative approach to identify unsafe older drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 1998;30:363–370. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Eberhard J. Safe Mobility of Senior Citizens. Journal of International Association of Traffic and Safety Sciences. 1996;20(1):29–37. [Google Scholar]
  • Evans L. Traffic Safety and the Driver. New York: van Nostrand Reinhold; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • Fildes BN, Corben BF, Kent SM, Oxley J, Le TM, Ryan P. Older road user crashes (Report No. 61) Australia: Monash University Accident Research Centre; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician” Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fox G, Bowden S, Smith D. On-road assessment of driving competence after brain impairment: Review of current practice and recommendations for a standardized examination. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1998;79:1288–1296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Galski T, Bruno RL, Ehle HT. Prediction of behind-the-wheel driving performance in patients with cerebral brain damage: A discrimminant function analysis. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 1993;47:391–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gstalter H, Fastenmeier W. Reliability of drivers in urban intersections. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2010;42(1):225–234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hunt LA, Murphy CF, Carr D, Duchek JM, Buckles V, Morris JC. Reliability of the Washington University Road Test. A performance-based assessment for drivers with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology. 1997;54:707–712. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Justiss MD. Development of a behind-the-wheel driving performance assessment for older drivers (Doctoral dissertation) University of Florida; America: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • Justiss MD, Stav W. Development of a Behind-the-Wheel Driving Performance Assessment for Older Adults. Topics in Geriatric Rehabilitation. 2006;22(2):121–128. [Google Scholar]
  • Katzman R, Brown T, Fuld P, Peck A, Schechter R, Schimmel H. Validation of a short Orientation-Memory-Concentration Test of cognitive impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1983;140(6):734–739. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Koppel S, Bohensky M, Langford J, Taranto D. Older Drivers, Crashes and Injuries. Traffic Injury Prevention. 2011;12(5):459–467. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Koppel S, Charlton JL, Langford J, Di Stefano M, Macdonald W, Mazer B, Gelinas I, Vrkljan B, Marshall S. Findings from the Candrive/Ozcandrive Study: A preliminary analysis of the Driving Observation Schedule (DOS) data; Paper presented at the Candrive/Ozcandrive Symposium, Australian Road Safety Research, Policy and Education Conference; Wellington New Zealand. 4th–6th October 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • Korner-Bitensky N, Bitensky J, Sofer S, Man-Son-Hing M, Gelinas I. Driving evaluation practices of clinicians working in the United States and Canada. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2006;60:428–434. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Langford J, Koppel S. Epidemiology of older driver crashes - identifying older driver risk factors and exposure patterns. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2006;9(5):309–321. [Google Scholar]
  • Langford J, Koppel S. Licence Restrictions as an Under-used Strategy in Managing Older Driver Safety. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2011;43(1):487–493. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Li G, Braver ER, Chen L. Fragility versus excessive crash involvement as determinants of high death rates per vehicle-mile of travel among older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2003;35(2):227–235. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lundberg C, Hakamies-Blomqvist L. Driving tests with older patients: effect of unfamiliar versus familiar vehicle. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2003;6(3):163–173. [Google Scholar]
  • Mackay M. Occupant protection and vehicle design. Paper presented at the Proceedings from the Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine Course on the Biomechanics of Impact Trauma; Los Angeles, USA. 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • Malloon K, Wood JM. Occupational therapy assessment of open-road driving performance: Validity of directed and self-directed navigational components. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2004;58:279–286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marshall S, Man-Son-Hing M, Bédard M, Charlton JL, Gagnon S, Gélinas I, Koppel S, Korner-Bitensky N, Langford J, Mazer B, Myers A, Naglie G, Polgar J, Porter M, Rapoport M, Tuokko H, Vrkljan B, Woolnough A. Protocol for Candrive II/Ozcandrive, a multicentre prospective older driver cohort study. Accident Analysis & Prevention: Special Issue (In Press). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McGwin G, Brown DB. Characteristics of traffic crashes among young, middle-aged and older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1999;31:181–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Michon JA. A critical view of driver behaviour models: What do we know, what should we do? In: Evans L, Schwing RC, editors. Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety. New York: Plenum Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • Moses JA., Jr Test review – Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004;19(5):703–708. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of American Geriatric Society. 2005;53(4):695–699. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Odenheimer GL, Beaudet M, Jette AM, Albert MS, Grande L, Minaker KL. Performance-based driving evaluation of the elderly driver: Safety, reliability, and validity. Journal of Gerontology. 1994;49:153–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • OECD . Ageing and Transport. Mobility needs and safety issues. Paris, France: OECD Publications; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • Padmanaban J. Crash injury experience of elderly drivers. Paper presented at the Aging and Driving Symposium, Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine; Des Plaines, IL. 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • Preusser D, Williams A, Ferguson S, Ulmer R, Weinstein H. Fatal crash risk for older drivers at intersections. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1998;30(2):151–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sivak M, Schoettle B. Recent Changes in the Age Composition of Drivers in 15 Countries. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • Smiley A. Adaptive strategies of older drivers. Paper presented at the Transportation in an Aging Society: A decade of Experience, Transportation Research Board; Maryland, USA. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • Staplin L, Gish KW, Wagner EK. MaryPODS revisited: Updated crash analysis and implications for screening program implementation. Journal of Safety Research. 2003;34(4):389–397. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Viano DC, Culver CC, Evans L, Frick M, Scott R. Involvement of older drivers in multi-vehicle side impact crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1990;22:177–188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • VicRoads . Pola Criteria: Version 4.2, July, 2005. Victoria, Australia: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • Vlahodimitrakou Z, Charlton JL, Langford J, Koppel S, Di Stefano M, Macdonald W, Smith G, Porter MM, Gelinas I, Mazer B, Vrkljan B, Marshall S. Characterisation of the Everyday Driving Behaviour of Older Drivers: Development of the Driving Observation Schedule (DOS) Accident Analysis & Prevention: Special Issue (In Press). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Willis SL. Driving competence: The person x environment fit. In: Schaie KW, Pietrucha M, editors. Mobility and Transportation in the elderly. New York: Springer; 2000. pp. 269–278. [Google Scholar]
  • Young KL, Salmon P, Lenne M. At the cross-roads: An on-road examination of driving errors at intersections. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2012. Availablie online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.09.014. [PubMed]